August 14, 2011

Religious Knowledge Deconstructed


For anyone interested, here is a paper I wrote recently about the possible evolutionary origins of religion. I'd love to hear your responses (if you can get through it).



The By-Product of Evolved Behaviours: Deconstructing Religious Knowledge

In this paper, I would like to explore how and why religious knowledge has been constructed. Why is unverifiable knowledge both ubiquitous and infused with value throughout human history? Why does religious truth inform billions of people about the way the universe functions and how is that truth reconciled with scientific knowledge? Please note that by religious knowledge or truth I mean information relating to entities or forces of a supernatural nature and everything attributed to arising from these entities. Due to evolutionary research, we know that human behaviour and the complex social knowledge collectively shared by us is the product of millions of years of natural selection. If natural selection, as Darwinian scientists claim, eliminates useless expenditure, how has the extravagant set of behaviours associated with religion survived? Darwinian selection dictates that animals that squander their time in wasteful activities will be outlived by animals that utilize their time efficiently, surviving and reproducing (Dawkins, 2006). The behaviour that religious knowledge encourages seems lavish and evolutionarily costly, like a peacock's tail. The resources used to construct a cathedral, for example are absurdly high in that the expense of human life, man hours and available material resources and seem indulgent for a building that is not used for sheltering or feeding a population. However, for a trait or a set of traits to survive, there must be a reason for them. The peacock's tail is for attracting mates. I will demonstrate how religious knowledge is simply a by-product of human behaviour that originally evolved for the purpose of survival but has since favoured the impulse for religion.

Firstly, I will show how the behaviours associated with the enhancement of social solidarity are formed and how they are predisposed to the creation of religious knowledge. These behaviours are effective communication of different models of reality and the application of commitment theory through: the demonstration of trustworthiness through irrational behaviour; the establishing of commitment through the sharing of emotions; and the public observation of rituals. Secondly, I will authenticate my claim that behaviours evolved for the purpose of individual rather than group survival value inclined humans toward forming religious knowledge. These behaviours are expressed as agency assignment, hyper agency detection device, and intentional stance. Thirdly, I will introduce some evolutionary psychological mechanisms and establish how they inform many aspects of religious knowledge, such as children's vulnerability to falsehoods, native dualism and teleology, irrationality associated with falling in love and the attachment system. Fourthly, I will draw comparisons between tendencies in animal behaviour toward man and the tendencies in human behaviour toward supernatural entities to illustrate how animal behaviour was a precursor to hominid behaviour. Lastly, I will outline the contemporary benefits of religious knowledge.

The most prevalent argument for the creation of religious knowledge as a by-product of certain environmentally dependent behaviours is the survival value associated with social bonding. It has been observed that religiously organized groups cooperate more effectively and have better success competitively than non-religiously organized groups (Dow, 2006). Apparently then, there is a correlation between cooperation and religious knowledge. There are several theories regarding how religious knowledge enhances group cohesiveness; one of them hinges on the idea that there is no absolute external reality and that the information we each process through our five senses is represented as different models of a reality (Dow, 2006). With the existence of variant internal models of reality, it was necessary for humans to develop a means of communicating effectively with each other using symbols. "Complex symbolic communication" (Dow, 2006, p. 69) provided humans with the ability to represent complicated ideas to others, thereby increasing the social cohesiveness of the group and ultimately its survival.

Communication is used by different organisms to indicate various ideas. In the non-human animal world signalling is a method of communicating reproductive fitness to potential mates. For humans, it was important to communicate trustworthiness in order to demonstrate an ability to cooperate in a group environment (Dow, 2006). This trustworthiness was manifested in "irrational acts of commitment" (Dow, 2006, p. 72). These irrational commitments to unverifiable knowledge (expressed by the commitment theory) created feelings of trust. If viewed from the perspective of a group aiming at survival, these seemingly irrational acts can be seen as rational. Groups that are the most cohesive tend to fare better in the long term than groups that do not excel at internal cooperation (Dow, 2006). So, religious knowledge is simultaneously rational, in that it promotes cooperation within a group and irrational, because it necessitates belief in supernatural beings. Akin to trustworthiness as an indicator of a willingness to cooperate, is the perspective from some religious minds that close-knit religious communities should maintain that closeness by imposing taboos. Restrictions regarding interaction with non-members of the group prevent individuals with an only casual commitment from diluting the intense feeling of unity the group shares.

Social commitment, as part of the commitment theory, also plays a part in the development of religious knowledge. Humans have an incredible capacity for "unprecedented level[s] of subjective commitment to social norms (Rossano, 2006, p. 348). Put simply, we have a tendency to want to conform. Self-conscious emotions like embarrassment, guilt, shame and pride are only meaningful when individuals are part of a group. The desire to conform in ways that will enhance interpersonal relationships compels individuals to behave in ways that are acceptable to the group. Having similar emotions with other members aids in building and maintaining social bonds (Rossano, 2006). Emotions such as guilt when letting others down, pride when exceeding others' expectations, empathy for those in pain or indignation against those who are perceived to be morally inferior, are powerful tools in simultaneously ensuring group cohesion and curbing the natural impulse to be self-interested. The social emotions humans display and have displayed historically are "harnessed" (Rossano, 2006, p. 348) to reinforce social unity and ostracize deviants.

Another important aspect involved in commitment theory is the observation of rituals. Certain patterns in animal behaviour have been viewed as a forerunner of religious ritualistic behaviour. Flocks of birds, for example, "perform their ritual evolutions in the sky" (Verkamp, 1991, p. 541) and could be perceived to be enacting meaningful customs. In humans, the practice and impact of rituals and ceremonies are far more easily seen. Traditional societies often engage in "rhythmic dancing, disturbing imagery, and the ingestion of psychotropic substances producing ecstatic emotional states that enhance social bonding" (Rossano, 2006, p. 348). Rituals such as these promote the release of brain opiates, which in turn aid in the construction of strong ties between members of a group. Initiation rituals performed in traditional societies also often involve strenuous physical and emotional trials. These trials demonstrate that an individual is willing to undergo hardship and is committed to the group (Rossano, 2006).

Commitment theories demonstrate the social pressures exerted on individuals attempting to maintain healthy group ties. Individual survival behaviours have also evolved over time, one of which, agency detection, is when an organism assigns another's actions to something like belief, desire or intention (Rossano, 2006). Studies show that, in humans, the ability to attribute agency to other people develops over the first several years of life. With such complex social dynamics as were exhibited in hominid culture, the capacity for comprehending the goals of others would have been a useful skill (Rossano, 2006). Members of groups who could accurately read the intentions of those around them would have a significant survival advantage.

In conjunction with the skill to assign agency in humans, the ability to see agency in inanimate objects and events was also an important survival skill for early humans and animals. This theory advances the idea of attributing mysterious circumstances to an agent and is called hyperactive agency detection device (HADD) (Sosis, 2009). Cognitive theorists contend that religions are brimming with knowledge of unseen agents such as gods and spirits, who cause things to happen (Dow, 2006). This modern belief may stem from an evolutionary trait exhibited in animals. For example, a rabbit that, seeing a rustle in the bushes, imagines a predator instead of what is most likely causing the rustle (the wind), has a greater chance of surviving than a rabbit who does not imagine a predator. One day the rustling will eventually be the sounds of a crouching tiger and the rabbit will have ensured its survival by running at the first sign of perceived danger.

A comparable theory, intentional stance, postulates that survival value is exhibited in the ability of organisms to speedily process information in dangerous situations and make decisions based on that information. It can be viewed as a short cut for second-guessing the intentions of entities that matter to us for purposes of survival (Dawkins, 2006). Similarly, animism is the "hyperactive habit of finding agency wherever anything puzzles or frightens us" (Ward, 2008, p. 222). After uncovering the fact that traditional societies subscribed to a belief in animism and other related religious concepts, anthropologists of the 19th century began to think of religious knowledge as culturally evolved from lower to higher levels of thought. The belief that spirits inhabit every animate and inanimate thing offended the European thinkers and they subsequently labeled traditional religious knowledge as savage and industrial religious knowledge as civilized (Dow, 2006). This mentality indicates a reluctance to accept a behavioural explanation for the evolution of religious knowledge. Fortunately, much of the research done today is less vulnerable to fallibilities of this kind.

Psychological mechanisms have contributed to the evolution of religious knowledge. One such mechanism that has persisted to this day is the facility with which children are "brainwashed" (Wright, 2009, p. 464) by perceived authority figures. In hominid societies as well as in contemporary societies, children must believe the council of their parents or risk endangering themselves. There is a selective advantage to children who are taught not to eat unidentified foods or venture near a cliff edge over those who learn from personal experience that falling very long distances can be fatal or that eating strange berries can cause bodily harm (Dawkins, 2006). It is an essential component of human survival to pass accumulated knowledge on to successive generations; however, the absolute trust a child places in his parents and elders leaves him vulnerable to a belief in falsehoods. A child cannot distinguish good advice from bad so when an elder admonishes him to "sacrifice a goat at the time of the full moon" (Dawkins, 2006, p. 205), the knowledge is stored and passed down to the next generation as valid. This gullibility is evolutionarily sound but in many cases prevents children from thinking rationally about the world and instills in them an alarming susceptibility to falsities that can persist into adulthood.

Another psychological mechanism that may prime human brains for the creation and acceptance of religious knowledge is the theory that dualism and teleology are ingrained in human thought processes. Dualism is the belief that the mind and body are separate and the body is inhabited by a spirit. Conversely, monists believe that "mind is a manifestation of matter" (Dawkins, 2006, p. 209) and cannot be separated from the body. These two convictions are important in regards to religious knowledge because one asserts the idea that a spirit can survive the body's death and live in an afterlife. Studies have been done that demonstrate an inclination for humans, and especially children, to support dualism rather than monism. Studies in the area of teleology have also been done to ascertain the predisposition of humans to accepting that philosophy. Teleology, the assignment of purpose to everything, was found to be widespread in children (Dawkins, 2006). We have already discussed the evolutionary purpose behind teleology; the contemporary effect is that we believe that since everything has a purpose, it must be the purpose of a supernatural power. The native nature of dualism and teleology in the human psyche indicates that humans may have a psychological predisposition for religion.

The irrationality of religion may be the by-product of a "particular built-in irrationality mechanism in the brain" (Dawkins, 2006, p. 214) which compels us to fall in love. This phenomenon drives most men and women to engage in monogamous relationships with partners that are not necessarily ideal. For example, a man is unlikely to find one woman a hundred times more lovable than her competitors, yet when he is in love, he often feels that this is so. The oddity of perpetuating a romantic relationship with only one person is exhibited in the fact that it is only in this particular area of life that monogamy is expected. It is socially acceptable to love more than one child, parent, friend, book or composer, but it is taboo to love more than one romantic partner. From the evolutionary standpoint of survival and reproduction, it seems beneficial to conceive a child and maintain a relationship with the other parent throughout the weaning process to ensure the survival of progeny. In regards to the production of religious knowledge, the irrationality mechanism in our brains could affect the human inclination to love supernatural beings or forces and to perform irrational acts motivated by that love (Dawkins, 2006).

Continuing with the theme of love, it has been contended that the attachment system is fundamental to religion, especially Christianity (Kirkpatrick, 2006). The attachment system, evolved in humans through natural selection, is meant to protect vulnerable infants from the dangers of their environment. If the primary caregiver of an infant is too far to provide necessary protection, the youngster will cry, call and reach to generate closer proximity. When the environment is safe, the role of the caregiver is to comfort and provide a "secure base" (Kirkpatrick, 2006, p. 5) from which the infant can explore. The attachment system may be a source of religious knowledge in that a supernatural being replaces the role of the primary caregiver in providing safety, comfort and security. For example, a religious person will go forward with faith that god will protect him from danger and sustain him through trials.

Some scientists and theologians have compared man's devotion to God with canine devotion to man, a source of religious knowledge founded on theories that animals' patterned or ritualistic behaviour anticipates human religious behaviour. Charles Darwin thought he found such patterns in the barking and growling of his dog at a parasol blowing in the wind (Verkamp, 1991). He hypothesized that this example displays elements of animism which has already been defined as the belief that supernatural forces explain all inexplicable movement. Darwin also saw significance in the loyalty of dogs to their masters and likened it to human devotion to God because the feelings characterized by it, "love, complete submission to an exalted and mysterious superior, a strong sense of dependence, fear, reverence, gratitude, and hope for the future" (Verkamp, 1991, p. 541) are similar to a dog's fear of and submission to its master. Further demonstrations of dog and man relationships versus man and god relationships are relevant because primitive man is closer in social behaviour to hunting dogs than he is to any other member of the animal kingdom (Verkamp, 1991). Hunting dogs become attached to human masters by transferring submissive attachment from the pack leader to a human. This is analogous to sudden conversions of many human adults to ways of perceiving god as all powerful and all knowing. Characterized by love, faithfulness and devotion, the transference of love exhibited by puppies for their mothers to human masters is also demonstrative of the love humans harbour for their supernatural masters.

The benefits of religious knowledge on the contemporary human population have less to do with survival and reproduction and more to do with social participation. For instance, the knowledge that religious groups have more social cohesion than other groups indicates that groups with similar beliefs, ideas and ways of life are proficient at cooperation (Dow, 2006). Socially, it is beneficial to belong to a group and to participate in activities that produce meaningful relationships. Some studies have shown that modern industrial societies that exhibit signs of religious faith are associated with longevity and greater health (Dow, 2006). Surely this is linked to the mental health attained through engaging in robust social interaction as opposed to solitary activities that tend to produce feelings of loneliness and separation from society. Another possible positive effect associated with religious knowledge is the placebo effect, resulting in reduced stress for those imbued with faith in a higher power. The placebo is related to the attachment system, in which a member of a religious group feels comforted and protected by a supernatural entity. This is also associated with the consolation theory, in which the faithful find the belief in a god and a heaven consoling.

Religious knowledge then, originated as a by-product of a set of behaviours that evolved through natural selection over the hundreds of thousands of years of human history. Originally intended to perform important roles in survival and reproduction, these behaviours have predisposed humans towards certain ways of producing knowledge, known as religion.

8 comments:

  1. Well done Tyger! You covered quite a few concepts with lots of examples. I'm very intrigued by the point you made in the 5th paragraph about self-conscious emotions. It had never occurred to me that the superego might be the evolutionary byproduct of working in groups! Cool stuff!

    ReplyDelete
  2. I really love researching this sort of thing. I find it edifying to approach an institution we often take for granted from such a different angle.

    ReplyDelete
  3. I can relate to your love of the search for research. However i believe God revealed His truths to Adam and succeeding prophets and the gospel truths were distorted and lost from there by the "wisemen" of the world; some deliberately changing the truths and some not understanding.
    GPMuses

    ReplyDelete
  4. Hey Tyger,
    I was doing some research on Pat Tillman after watching that documentary last night, and in doing so I came across this essay by Sam Harris. While it's off topic (in that it deals with the political and ethical implications of religion in society), I found it thoroughly enjoyable and relevant enough to post it here.

    Enjoy!
    http://www.truthdig.com/dig/item/200512_an_atheist_manifesto/

    ReplyDelete
  5. Thanks for your comment Dad. But what do you say to the arguments presented here? Do you think they have any validity whatsoever?

    ReplyDelete
  6. I think you made a lot of really good arguments! I particularly agree with your points about social groups, trust, and children being exposed at an early age to the "accumulated knowledge" before being able to form their own opinions and decisions -- and how they all played a fairly significant evolutionary / survival role.

    ReplyDelete
  7. Thanks for reading, Mark! Isn't it interesting to think about how and why humans are the way they are today and the intricacies involved in that process?

    ReplyDelete
  8. An interesting paper, Becca, but, as I'm sure you know, I don't agree with it. Your arguments are presented very concisely but they are leaving out a very important assumption - what if there is a higher force out there that has greater power/knowledge/ability than we do? What if this higher force has the ability to influence human thought and action? Your paper assumes that we (human scientists) know all the sources of power and influence in our universe - a very big assumption. Your arguments are sound if based on the idea that there is nothing greater or more knowledgeable than humankind - nothing more than evolution and humans ourselves that can influence humankind. But what if there is?
    Jennie

    ReplyDelete